Abstract

The left superior temporal sulcus (STS) plays an important role in integrating audiovisual information and is functionally connected to disparate regions of the brain. For the integration of gesture information in an abstract sentence context (metaphoric gestures), intact connectivity between the left STS and the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) should be important. Patients with schizophrenia have problems with the processing of metaphors (concretism) and show aberrant structural connectivity of long fiber bundles. Thus, we tested the hypothesis that patients with schizophrenia differ in the functional connectivity of the left STS to the IFG for the processing of metaphoric gestures. During functional magnetic resonance imaging data acquisition, 16 patients with schizophrenia (P) and a healthy control group (C) were shown videos of an actor performing gestures in a concrete (iconic, IC) and abstract (metaphoric, MP) sentence context. A psychophysiological interaction analysis based on the seed region from a previous analysis in the left STS was performed. In both groups we found common positive connectivity for IC and MP of the STS seed region to the left middle temporal gyrus (MTG) and left ventral IFG. The interaction of group (C>P) and gesture condition (MP>IC) revealed effects in the connectivity to the bilateral IFG and the left MTG with patients exhibiting lower connectivity for the MP condition. In schizophrenia the left STS is misconnected to the IFG, particularly during the processing of MP gestures. Dysfunctional integration of gestures in an abstract sentence context might be the basis of certain interpersonal communication problems in the patients.

Introduction

Gestures are an important component of interpersonal communication1 and have a large impact on speech perception,2,3 memory,4,5 and social functioning.6–8 Patients with schizophrenia, however, show impairments in the perception and production of arm and hand gestures.7,9–15

The processing of speech-related gestures involves different modalities, ie, the combination of auditory and visual input. The ability to combine information from multiple sensory modalities into a single, unified percept is essential for a person interacting with the external world16 and necessary for the processing of multimodal communication. However, multimodal integration requires a transfer of information, ie, functional connectivity, between disparate regions of the brain.17–19 It has been shown that the area around the left posterior superior temporal sulcus (STS; including parts of the middle and superior temporal gyrus; MTG/STG) plays an important role in integrating audiovisual information, eg, from speech and gestures.20–23

Patients with schizophrenia have difficulties in the multimodal integration of sensory input,24–29 which we hypothesize to be based on a dysfunctional connectivity of the left posterior temporal lobe. Patients with schizophrenia show impaired structural30–33 and functional brain connectivity,34–42 specifically between superior temporal and frontal brain regions (see Wolf et al43 for a review). However, up to now the functional neural connectivity of the left STS during speech and gesture integration in schizophrenia has not received attention.

Different types of gestures vary in their relation to language,1 and consequently differ with regard to integration effort and the involvement of different regions of the brain in their processing.22,23,44 Iconic gestures refer to the concrete content of sentences, whereas metaphoric gestures illustrate abstract information. For example, in the sentence “The politician builds a bridge to the next topic” (depicting an arch with the hand) abstract content is illustrated by a metaphoric gesture. However, the same gesture (depicting an arch with the hand) can be iconic when paired with the sentence “There is a bridge over the river,” as iconic gestures illustrate concrete physical features of the world.22 The processing of iconic and metaphoric gestures in healthy subjects has been related to the left STS (including MTG and STG),20,21 with the left IFG being additionally activated during metaphoric gesture processing.44

Patients with schizophrenia show difficulties in the understanding of figurative aspects of speech such as metaphors, proverbs, humor or abstract concepts,45,46 clinically termed concretism. It has been shown that severity of concretism in patients with schizophrenia is negatively correlated with activation in the left IFG during processing of metaphors.46 Previously the processing of concrete and abstract sentences accompanied by gestures has been compared in patients with schizophrenia.47 Intact integration processes were found for gestures in a concrete sentence context (iconic gestures), reflected by activation around the left STS. By contrast, patients demonstrated no activation increase for the integration of gesture meaning into an abstract sentence context (metaphoric gestures) in contrast to control conditions, a process that activated the left STS and the left IFG in healthy subjects.4,22,44 Thus, besides the assumption of a general dysfunction in gesture production and comprehension in patients with schizophrenia,7,13–15,48 these data suggest that integration processes related to abstract utterances accompanied by gestures are specifically impaired in schizophrenia.

Because disconnectivity of the left posterior STS might be the basis of dysfunctional processing of gestures in an abstract sentence context, we tested the hypothesis that patients with schizophrenia demonstrate a reduced functional connectivity of the left STS to the IFG for the processing of metaphoric gestures. A psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis based on a seed region identified in a previous analysis (located in an area around the left posterior STS)47 has been performed. A similar region of the temporal lobe has been previously reported to be structurally49 and functionally disconnected in schizophrenia42 or high risk populations50 and relevant for integration of speech and gesture.20,21 With this PPI analysis differences in connectivity between groups (patients vs controls) and conditions (gestures in a concrete [iconic, IC] vs abstract [metaphoric, MP] sentence context) have been disentangled.

Methods

Participants

Patients.

Sixteen patients (6 female) diagnosed with schizophrenia (F20; mean age = 38 years, range: 19–53 years) participated in this and in a previous study.47 Patients were recruited from and diagnosed by independent clinicians according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) criteria at the Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Medical School, RWTH Aachen University, Germany.

All participants were free of visual and auditory deficits, additional neurological and medical impairments as well as any cerebral abnormality, as assessed by a T1-weighted MRI. All patients reported that they were right handed,51 and German was their primary language. Symptom ratings in patients were recorded using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS).52

Clinical Characteristics.

Patients were moderately ill with an average total PANSS score of 64 (SD = 19), with positive subscore of 16 (SD = 8), negative subscore of 16 (SD = 7) and general subscore of 32 (SD = 9). All patients were on stable doses of atypical antipsychotic medication (mean dose in Haloperidol equivalents = 9.62mg/day, SD = 4.26).53

Healthy Control Group.

Sixteen right-handed healthy male controls participated in this and previous studies,22,47 all native German speakers (mean age = 27.9 years, range: 19–47 years) with no impairments of vision or hearing. None of the participants had any medical, neurological or psychiatric illness, past or present.

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the RWTH Aachen University. All participants gave written informed consent and were paid 20 Euro for participation.

Stimulus Construction

The stimulus material consisted of video clips (each with a duration of 5 s) that presented a male actor performing different combinations of speech and gestures (for detailed description, see Straube et al22,47 and figure 1). The gestures in the iconic condition (IC) illustrated the form, size or movement of something concrete that was mentioned in the accompanying speech,1 whereas those in the metaphoric condition (MP) illustrated the form, size or movement of something abstract that was mentioned in the associated speech.1 An isolated gesture (G) and isolated sentence condition (S, visible speaking actor) were included to explore unimodal from bimodal speech/gesture effects at the neural level. Sentences used in this paradigm were easy to understand and conventional to ensure that comprehension is feasible in patients with schizophrenia.47

Fig. 1.

Examples of the experimental conditions.22,47

The stimulus material consisted of videos of an actor performing iconic (IC) and metaphoric (MP) coverbal gestures and corresponding concrete and abstract sentences, respectively. For illustrative purposes the spoken German sentences were translated into English and written in speech bubbles. Concrete sentences without gesture (S, visible speaking actor) and gestures without speech (G) were additionally presented.

Experimental Design

Thirty stimuli from each of the experimental conditions (see figure 1) were presented in a block design.22,47 Each block consisted of 5 videos of the same condition and was 25 seconds in length (5 videos × 5 s). In total, 6 blocks of each condition were presented in a pseudorandomized order and separated by a baseline condition (gray background with a fixation cross) with a duration of 15 seconds, during which the fixation cross shortly disappeared 2 times (about every 5 s). In total, each participant saw 120 video clips that lasted 17 minutes.

Subjects were instructed to watch the videos and to respond each time they saw a new picture appear (either the video or baseline fixation cross) by pressing a button with the left index finger (see Straube et al22,47). This was done to ensure that they paid attention during all conditions and baseline. This implicit encoding task was chosen to focus participants’ attention on the middle of the screen and it enabled us to investigate implicit speech and gesture processing. Presentation software (Version 9.2, 2005) was used for stimulus presentation and response measurement in the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiment.

fMRI Data Acquisition

MRI was performed on a 1.5T Philips scanner (Philips MRT Achieva series). Functional data were acquired with echo planar images in 31 transversal slices (repetition time [TR] = 2800ms; echo time [TE] = 50ms; flip angle = 90°; slice thickness = 3.5mm; interslice gap = 0.35mm; field of view = 240mm; voxel resolution = 3.75 × 3.75mm). Slices were positioned to achieve whole brain coverage. Three hundred and sixty volumes were acquired during the functional run. The experimental setup including the paradigm, scanner, and scanning parameters used in the current study was identical to the one previously published.22,47

Data Analysis

MR images were analyzed using standard routines for first and second level analyses in Statistical Parametric Mapping 8 (SPM8; www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk, accessed July 28, 2013) implemented in MATLAB 7 (Mathworks Inc). The first 5 volumes of each functional run were discarded from the analysis in order to minimize T1-saturation effects. To correct for different acquisition times, the signal measured in each slice was shifted relative to the acquisition time of the middle slice using a slice interpolation in time. All images of one session were realigned to the first image of a run to correct for head movement and normalized into standard stereotactic anatomical Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) brain space by using the transformation matrix calculated from the first echo planar imaging (EPI) scan of each subject and the EPI template. Afterwards, the normalized data with a resliced voxel size of 4×4 × 4mm were smoothed with a 10mm full width at the half maximum isotropic Gaussian kernel to accommodate for intersubject variation in brain anatomy. A high-pass filter (128 s cut of period) was applied to remove low frequency fluctuations in the blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal.

For the regular analyses (see Straube et al47) the expected hemodynamic response at the onset of each block was modeled with a duration of 22 seconds by a canonical hemodynamic response function.47 The function was convolved with the block sequence in a general linear model, which also included the 6 movement nuisance regressors to account for movement during the scan interval. These previous analyses were used as basis to extract individual activation time courses of the left posterior STS and to build up the PPI analyses.

PPI Analyses of the Left STS Cluster

A PPI analysis was conducted to assess differences in neural connectivity of the area around the left STS (including parts of the MTG and STG) for groups and conditions. Standard routines of SPM8 as described in the corresponding manual were used to set up the PPI analysis. For this analysis we specified as seed region of interest (ROI) the STS cluster of 14 voxels resulting from the common activation for patients and control subjects for the processing of iconic coverbal gestures (see Straube et al47 and below). For the PPI analysis the block design described above and used previously to assess group differences in activation amplitude47 was employed on first level including the 2 conditions of interest (IC and MP) and the 2 isolated conditions (S and G). BOLD responses over time of all voxels within the ROI were extracted for each subject as cluster-eigenvariates. The volumes of interest created this way included each subject’s beta values in the STS over time. In the next step, those time courses were used to create the PPIs. For each condition, 1 PPI was calculated, resulting in 4 PPIs for each single subject. The new first-level model was then specified implementing the general activation change within the area around the STS over time and each PPI as regressors for the respective condition. To control for condition specific differences in amplitude and movement, the conditions and regressors of the previously defined block design (see above) were additionally included in the model. Baseline t-contrasts set up on the respective regressors (PPIs) were performed for each single condition to assess correlations in neural response with activity in the STS with respect to each single condition. On the group level, a random effects analysis (full factorial analysis) was conducted, using the corresponding contrast images from each participant and patient, in order to assess effects of condition and group on STS connectivity. Age, medication, intelligence quotient (IQ), and STS activation amplitude had no significant effect on the connectivity results (see Supplementary Data).

Seed Region

As seed region, we used the overlap in activation for patients and controls in response to the iconic gesture conditions (IC) contrasted to unimodal control conditions (speech, S; gesture, G; ([(P-IC>P-S ∩ P-IC>P-G) inclusively masked by P-S ∩ P-G] ∩ [(C-IC>C-S ∩ C-IC>C-G) inclusively masked by C-S ∩ C-G]; see Straube et al47); note that reaction times have not been used as covariate in the current analysis because it had only a minimal effect on t values and cluster sizes). Using this contrast (reflecting integration of the IC conditions), we found an overlap of conjunctional activation for patients and control subjects in a cluster located around the left posterior STS with 3 submaxima (MTG: MNIx,y,z = [−60, −56, 12], t = 1.96; MTG: MNIx,y,z = [−52, −56, 12], t = 1.97; STG: MNIx,y,z = [−56, −48, 16], t = 1.76; number of voxels = 14; P < .05).

To correct the fMRI results for multiple comparisons we employed a Monte-Carlo simulation of the brain volume to establish an appropriate voxel contiguity threshold.22,54 Assuming an individual voxel type I error of P < .05, a cluster extent of 17 contiguous resembled voxels was indicated as necessary to correct for multiple voxel comparisons at P < .05. We applied this cluster threshold to all reported analyses (this is the same procedure and threshold as previously applied to the identical paradigm in healthy subjects22 and patients with schizophrenia).47

The reported voxel coordinates of activation peaks are located in MNI space. The functional data were referenced to probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps for the anatomical localization.55

Statistical analyses of data other than fMRI were performed using SPSS version 15.0 for Windows (SPSS Inc).

Contrasts of Interest

We computed particular contrasts of interest in order to test our hypotheses. First, we analysed common connectivity patterns across groups and conditions (C-IC ∩ C-MP ∩ P-IC ∩ P-MP) to detect similarities in functional connectivity between groups and conditions.

In a second step we performed interaction analyses of group by condition to test our hypothesis that patients and controls would differ in STS connectivity predominantly in the metaphoric gesture condition. Interaction analyses were calculated in both directions ([C-MP>C-IC]>[P-MP>P-IC] and [P-MP>P-IC]>[C-MP>C-IC]).

For additionally exploratory analyses and illustrations of the individual connectivity patterns for each group and condition see Supplementary Data.

Results

For the processing of iconic as well as metaphoric gestures in both patients and control subjects ([C-IC ∩ C-MP ∩ P-IC ∩ P-MP]; seefigure 2) we found the left IFG (p. Orbitalis; MNIx,y,z = [−48, 24, −4], t = 2.59, number of voxels = 32; P < .05 corrected) and MTG (MNIx,y,z = [−56, −56, 12], t = 4.66, number of voxels = 52; P < .05 corrected) commonly connected to the seed region (STS). Connectivity to the left IFG was negatively correlated to positive symptoms (PANSS pos. scale; Spearmans’ rho = −0.487, P < .032, 1-tailed), suggesting increasing STS-IFG connectivity in patients goes along with a reduction in positive symptom scores. We did not find connectivity correlations for PANSS negative or general subscores.

For the interaction of group and condition ((C-MP>C-IC)>(P-MP>P-IC)) we obtained a widespread network of regions including the bilateral frontal, parietal, and left temporal structures as well as the anterior cingulate cortex and subcortical regions (see figure 3 and table 1). Bar graphs in figure 3 indicate that healthy controls but not patients demonstrate an increased connectivity to the left STS in these regions for the processing of metaphoric in contrast to iconic gestures. The opposite interaction contrast ((P-MP>P-IC)>(C-MP>C-IC)) revealed activity in the bilateral lingual gyrus (MNIx,y,z = [−8, −60, −4], t = 3.12, 77 voxels) the right hippocampus (MNIx,y,z = [16, −12, −16], t = 3.21, 52 voxels), the right inferior temporal gyrus (MNIx,y,z = [36, 0, −12], t = 2.30, 20 voxels), the left middle orbitofrontal gyrus (MNIx,y,z = [−32, 44, −12], t = 3.26, 17 voxels), and the left precuneus (MNIx,y,z = [0, −56, 32], t = 2.55, 60 voxels; all P < .05 corrected), indicating increased connectivity in these regions for patients for the processing of metaphoric gestures.

Table 1.

Interaction of Group (C>P) and Gesture Condition (MP>IC)

Anatomical RegionCluster ExtentCoordinatest ValueP ValueNumber of Voxels
xyz
Right MFGRight SFG, IFG pars Tri.  36  36  403.77<.001691
Right precentral gyrus, SFG, MFG  36−8  643.19.001
Right SFG, MCC  12  32  443.19.001
Left parahippocampal gyrusLeft cerebellum, HC−8−20−283.62<.001339
Right cerebellum, lingual gyrus  12−36−163.13.001
Left MTG, STS, HC−40−44  43.01.002
Right ACCLeft ACC; right nucleus caudatus  8  24  203.72<.001221
Left thalamus, pallidum, HC−12−12  03.30.001
Bilateral nucleus caudatus; left pallidum  0  4  82.62.005
Left MFGLeft IFG pars Tri., SFG−28  32  282.93.002122
Left IFG pars Oper./Tri., precentral gyrus−32  8  242.72.004
Left IFG pars Tri./Oper., insula−44  24  82.59.005
Left middle occipital gyrusLeft calcarine gyrus, MTG−32−80  123.13.00158
Left middle Occ. gyr., angular gyr., MTG−32−64  202.69.004
Right thalamusRight insula, Heschls gyrus  28−24  122.78.00350
Right precuneus, PCC, HC  20−40  162.60.005
Left SFGLeft MFG, precentral gyrus−28−4  682.44.00845
Left MFG, SFG, SMA−24  12  602.22.014
Left IPLLeft postcentral gyrus, angular gyrus−40−44  442.33.01135
Left IPL, supramarginal gyrus, angular gyrus−52−48  362.24.014
Right precuneusRight postcentral gyrus, IPL  20−48  482.63.00526
Right postcentral/precentral gyrus, SMA  24−32  561.99.024
Right MCC, paracentral lobe, precuneus  12−40  481.77.039
Left SFGLeft SFG; Right ACC−4  48  282.11.01824
Right ACC, MCC, SFG  8  44  282.07.020
Left MFG, SFG−20  40  201.82.035
Right paracentral lobeRight postcentral/precentral gyrus  12−28  642.80.00321
Left HCLeft thalamus, PCC−20−36  122.39.00920
Right ITGRight fusiform gyrus, temporal pole  40−4−363.80<.00119
Anatomical RegionCluster ExtentCoordinatest ValueP ValueNumber of Voxels
xyz
Right MFGRight SFG, IFG pars Tri.  36  36  403.77<.001691
Right precentral gyrus, SFG, MFG  36−8  643.19.001
Right SFG, MCC  12  32  443.19.001
Left parahippocampal gyrusLeft cerebellum, HC−8−20−283.62<.001339
Right cerebellum, lingual gyrus  12−36−163.13.001
Left MTG, STS, HC−40−44  43.01.002
Right ACCLeft ACC; right nucleus caudatus  8  24  203.72<.001221
Left thalamus, pallidum, HC−12−12  03.30.001
Bilateral nucleus caudatus; left pallidum  0  4  82.62.005
Left MFGLeft IFG pars Tri., SFG−28  32  282.93.002122
Left IFG pars Oper./Tri., precentral gyrus−32  8  242.72.004
Left IFG pars Tri./Oper., insula−44  24  82.59.005
Left middle occipital gyrusLeft calcarine gyrus, MTG−32−80  123.13.00158
Left middle Occ. gyr., angular gyr., MTG−32−64  202.69.004
Right thalamusRight insula, Heschls gyrus  28−24  122.78.00350
Right precuneus, PCC, HC  20−40  162.60.005
Left SFGLeft MFG, precentral gyrus−28−4  682.44.00845
Left MFG, SFG, SMA−24  12  602.22.014
Left IPLLeft postcentral gyrus, angular gyrus−40−44  442.33.01135
Left IPL, supramarginal gyrus, angular gyrus−52−48  362.24.014
Right precuneusRight postcentral gyrus, IPL  20−48  482.63.00526
Right postcentral/precentral gyrus, SMA  24−32  561.99.024
Right MCC, paracentral lobe, precuneus  12−40  481.77.039
Left SFGLeft SFG; Right ACC−4  48  282.11.01824
Right ACC, MCC, SFG  8  44  282.07.020
Left MFG, SFG−20  40  201.82.035
Right paracentral lobeRight postcentral/precentral gyrus  12−28  642.80.00321
Left HCLeft thalamus, PCC−20−36  122.39.00920
Right ITGRight fusiform gyrus, temporal pole  40−4−363.80<.00119

Note: ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; HC, hippocampus; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; IPL, superior parietal lobe; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus; MCC, middle cingulate cortex; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; Pars Orb., Pars orbitalis; Pars Tri., Pars triangularis; PCC, posterior cingulate gyrus; Rol. OP, Rolandic operculum; SMA, supplemental motor area; SFG, superior frontal gyrus; SPL, superior parietal lobe; STG, superior temporal gyrus.

Coordinates (in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space), cluster extent and t values of the interaction of group by condition.

Cluster level corrected at P < .05.

Table 1.

Interaction of Group (C>P) and Gesture Condition (MP>IC)

Anatomical RegionCluster ExtentCoordinatest ValueP ValueNumber of Voxels
xyz
Right MFGRight SFG, IFG pars Tri.  36  36  403.77<.001691
Right precentral gyrus, SFG, MFG  36−8  643.19.001
Right SFG, MCC  12  32  443.19.001
Left parahippocampal gyrusLeft cerebellum, HC−8−20−283.62<.001339
Right cerebellum, lingual gyrus  12−36−163.13.001
Left MTG, STS, HC−40−44  43.01.002
Right ACCLeft ACC; right nucleus caudatus  8  24  203.72<.001221
Left thalamus, pallidum, HC−12−12  03.30.001
Bilateral nucleus caudatus; left pallidum  0  4  82.62.005
Left MFGLeft IFG pars Tri., SFG−28  32  282.93.002122
Left IFG pars Oper./Tri., precentral gyrus−32  8  242.72.004
Left IFG pars Tri./Oper., insula−44  24  82.59.005
Left middle occipital gyrusLeft calcarine gyrus, MTG−32−80  123.13.00158
Left middle Occ. gyr., angular gyr., MTG−32−64  202.69.004
Right thalamusRight insula, Heschls gyrus  28−24  122.78.00350
Right precuneus, PCC, HC  20−40  162.60.005
Left SFGLeft MFG, precentral gyrus−28−4  682.44.00845
Left MFG, SFG, SMA−24  12  602.22.014
Left IPLLeft postcentral gyrus, angular gyrus−40−44  442.33.01135
Left IPL, supramarginal gyrus, angular gyrus−52−48  362.24.014
Right precuneusRight postcentral gyrus, IPL  20−48  482.63.00526
Right postcentral/precentral gyrus, SMA  24−32  561.99.024
Right MCC, paracentral lobe, precuneus  12−40  481.77.039
Left SFGLeft SFG; Right ACC−4  48  282.11.01824
Right ACC, MCC, SFG  8  44  282.07.020
Left MFG, SFG−20  40  201.82.035
Right paracentral lobeRight postcentral/precentral gyrus  12−28  642.80.00321
Left HCLeft thalamus, PCC−20−36  122.39.00920
Right ITGRight fusiform gyrus, temporal pole  40−4−363.80<.00119
Anatomical RegionCluster ExtentCoordinatest ValueP ValueNumber of Voxels
xyz
Right MFGRight SFG, IFG pars Tri.  36  36  403.77<.001691
Right precentral gyrus, SFG, MFG  36−8  643.19.001
Right SFG, MCC  12  32  443.19.001
Left parahippocampal gyrusLeft cerebellum, HC−8−20−283.62<.001339
Right cerebellum, lingual gyrus  12−36−163.13.001
Left MTG, STS, HC−40−44  43.01.002
Right ACCLeft ACC; right nucleus caudatus  8  24  203.72<.001221
Left thalamus, pallidum, HC−12−12  03.30.001
Bilateral nucleus caudatus; left pallidum  0  4  82.62.005
Left MFGLeft IFG pars Tri., SFG−28  32  282.93.002122
Left IFG pars Oper./Tri., precentral gyrus−32  8  242.72.004
Left IFG pars Tri./Oper., insula−44  24  82.59.005
Left middle occipital gyrusLeft calcarine gyrus, MTG−32−80  123.13.00158
Left middle Occ. gyr., angular gyr., MTG−32−64  202.69.004
Right thalamusRight insula, Heschls gyrus  28−24  122.78.00350
Right precuneus, PCC, HC  20−40  162.60.005
Left SFGLeft MFG, precentral gyrus−28−4  682.44.00845
Left MFG, SFG, SMA−24  12  602.22.014
Left IPLLeft postcentral gyrus, angular gyrus−40−44  442.33.01135
Left IPL, supramarginal gyrus, angular gyrus−52−48  362.24.014
Right precuneusRight postcentral gyrus, IPL  20−48  482.63.00526
Right postcentral/precentral gyrus, SMA  24−32  561.99.024
Right MCC, paracentral lobe, precuneus  12−40  481.77.039
Left SFGLeft SFG; Right ACC−4  48  282.11.01824
Right ACC, MCC, SFG  8  44  282.07.020
Left MFG, SFG−20  40  201.82.035
Right paracentral lobeRight postcentral/precentral gyrus  12−28  642.80.00321
Left HCLeft thalamus, PCC−20−36  122.39.00920
Right ITGRight fusiform gyrus, temporal pole  40−4−363.80<.00119

Note: ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; HC, hippocampus; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; IPL, superior parietal lobe; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus; MCC, middle cingulate cortex; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; Pars Orb., Pars orbitalis; Pars Tri., Pars triangularis; PCC, posterior cingulate gyrus; Rol. OP, Rolandic operculum; SMA, supplemental motor area; SFG, superior frontal gyrus; SPL, superior parietal lobe; STG, superior temporal gyrus.

Coordinates (in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space), cluster extent and t values of the interaction of group by condition.

Cluster level corrected at P < .05.

Discussion

Interpersonal communication and especially speech-gesture interactions have been increasingly investigated in patients with schizophrenia.9–13,15,48,56–58 Despite the available data suggesting that patients have deficits in the comprehension of communicative gestures7,9,15 as well as in the understanding of abstract sentences (“concretism”),46,59,60 the neural mechanisms underlying these dysfunctions are yet unknown. Here we showed that patients demonstrate a specific functional disconnectivity of the left STS and the IFG bilaterally during the processing of metaphoric gestures. This disconnectivity might be the basis of dysfunctional integration of gesture information into an abstract sentence context and thus an underlying reason for interpersonal communication problems in patients.

The left posterior STS is an important multimodal integration site61,62 relevant for the integration of speech and gesture information.20–23,44,63 Dysfunctional visual64 and multisensory integration in patients with schizophrenia has already been reported.24,25 Previously however, we found that the area around the left STS in patients with schizophrenia was activated to the same extent as in healthy control subjects when iconic coverbal gestures were contrasted to unimodal control conditions (speech only, gesture only).47 For the integration of metaphoric gestures it is important to build an abstract relation between concrete visual and abstract verbal information. For building this relation, additional online integration or unification processes in distantly located regions of the brain seem to be relevant.22,44,47 In line with the disconnectivity hypotheses of schizophrenia, we found a functional disconnectivity of the left posterior STS to the prefrontal cortex as potential basis for the aberrant processing of gestures in an abstract sentence context.

Considering the reported dysfunctions in gesture production and gesture imitation (eg, Matthews et al10 and Park et al12), it is justified to assume a common basis/etiology of impairments in gesture perception and production. This assumption is also in line with the “mirror neuron theory”65 that assumes the same neural basis for action execution and action perception/understanding. Thus, dysfunctional processing and understanding of gestures might be related to a gesture production deficit in general or could even be a direct result of impaired imitation learning in patients during development.10

In our study we found a positive connectivity of the left STS to the left IFG and left MTG for both groups and for gestures accompanying concrete as well as abstract utterances (see figure 2), indicating basically intact connectivity in patients with schizophrenia between the most relevant language regions. This is in line with the finding that patients generally understood the verbal utterances (for behavioral data see Straube et al47 and Supplementary Data), but differentially processed the gestures in abstract sentence context.47

However, despite these communalities we found a decrease of connectivity for the processing of metaphoric gestures in a widespread network of regions including bilateral frontal, parietal, and left temporal structures in patients compared with controls (see figure 3). The data of the healthy control group suggest that a high connectivity between these regions and the left STS is important for the integration of gestures in an abstract sentence context. In particular, the left STS is part of Wernicke’s and the left IFG part of Broca’s language areas, which are predominantly connected through the arcuate fasciculus (eg, Abdul-Rahman et al30, Bernal et al66, and Douaud et al67). In schizophrenia abnormalities in these structural connections have been reported,30,31,33,67–70 which might be the basis of impairments in functional connectivity between frontal and temporal brain regions.37,71 Thus, patients with schizophrenia fail to use these distributed brain resources appropriately for the processing of metaphoric coverbal gestures. In addition to the hypothesized disconnectivity between left STS and IFG regions, we found the most prominent group differences in connectivity between the left STS and regions of the right frontal cortex. Activity of the right IFG in healthy subjects has been reported for the processing of unrelated speech and gesture information.4 Thus, in healthy subjects the right hemisphere might contribute to the integration processes of the left STS by helping to interpret concrete gesture information in an abstract way. Therefore, our finding of a reduced connectivity between the left STS and the right frontal cortex is in line with the hypothesis that the core deficit in psychosis is a failure in an adequate utilization of right and left hemispheric functions72 in a context dependent manner.

In addition to these findings, we also observed increased connectivity between the STS and the bilateral lingual gyrus, the right hippocampus, the right inferior temporal gyrus, the left middle orbitofrontal gyrus, and the precuneus in patients for the processing of metaphoric gestures. Whereas most studies have found reduced connectivity in schizophrenia,73,74 other demonstrated increased connectivity75,76 or a rather mixed pattern of increased and decreased connections.77 Increased connectivity in our study might suggest compensation mechanisms or the application of different processing strategies (eg, memory retrieval reflected in hippocampal activity). However, further research is necessary to understand task and region specific aberrant connectivity of patients with schizophrenia.

This study has to be interpreted in light of some limitations. First, the sample of patients was relatively ill and findings might not be generalizable to a more remitted sample. Second, the study was focused on specific gesture types suitable for the current research question. However, other gesture types such as deictic or beat gestures might also be impaired in patients with schizophrenia. Third, due to our specific paradigm it is difficult to infer whether patients have in fact problems with gesture integration or rather with the processing of an abstract context itself. However, results of the gesture only condition indicate already a reduced connectivity for the processing of gestures alone (see bar graphs figures 2 and 3). This disconnectivity seems to be increased for the processing of gestures in the abstract sentence context and reduced for the processing of gestures in the concrete sentence context (Supplementary Data), supporting the interaction of speech and gesture processing in the current experiment. Finally, several other factors, such as negative symptoms,10 age,78 IQ or working memory,10 might influence gesture perception and production. Therefore future studies should investigate more specifically the role of those other sources of variance relevant for speech-gesture integration performance and STS connectivity in patients with schizophrenia.

Fig. 2.

Common positive connectivity for patients and controls across gesture conditions. Commonalties in STS connectivity between patients and controls across gesture conditions (C-IC ∩ C-MP ∩ P-IC ∩ P-MP). IC, iconic gesture condition; MP, metaphoric gesture condition; G, gesture only; S, speech only (no gesture, but visible speaking actor).

For the first time, this study investigated the connectivity of the posterior STS in patients with schizophrenia while processing illustrative and elaborative gestures that differed in their relation to speech. Our data indicate a reduced connectivity in patients, between brain regions normally involved in the processing of gestures in an abstract sentence context. It seems that processes (such as in perceptual matching)47 that require the connection of brain regions in close proximity are intact in patients, whereas processes relying on the interplay of more distant regions (as here observed for understanding higher order relations)47 are disturbed most dramatically. Thus, the disconnectivity between STS and IFG might be the basis of dysfunctional integration of metaphoric coverbal gestures and related interpersonal communication problems in patients with schizophrenia.

Funding

Interdisciplinary Center for Clinical Research “IZKF BIOMAT,” Aachen, Germany (IZKF VV N68); the German Research Foundation (DFG: Ki 588/6-1); von Behring-Röntgen-Stiftung (project no. 59-0002). B.S. is supported by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF; project no. 01GV0615).

Acknowledgments

The data were collected in Aachen (K.S., A.G., and B.S.), and data analyses and manuscript writing were performed in Marburg (T.K. and B.S.). The authors have declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study.

References

1.

McNeill
D
, ed.
Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal About Thought
.
Chicago, IL
:
University of Chicago Press
;
1992
.

2.

Kelly
SD
Ozyürek
A
Maris
E
.
Two sides of the same coin: speech and gesture mutually interact to enhance comprehension
.
Psychol Sci
.
2010
;
21
:
260
267
.

3.

Wu
YC
Coulson
S
.
Gestures modulate speech processing early in utterances
.
Neuroreport
.
2010
;
21
:
522
526
.

4.

Straube
B
Green
A
Weis
S
Chatterjee
A
Kircher
T
.
Memory effects of speech and gesture binding: cortical and hippocampal activation in relation to subsequent memory performance
.
J Cogn Neurosci
.
2009
;
21
:
821
836
.

5.

Straube
B
Green
A
Chatterjee
A
Kircher
T
.
Encoding social interactions: the neural correlates of true and false memories
.
J Cogn Neurosci
.
2011
;
23
:
306
324
.

6.

Straube
B
Green
A
Jansen
A
Chatterjee
A
Kircher
T
.
Social cues, mentalizing and the neural processing of speech accompanied by gestures
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2010
;
48
:
382
393
.

7.

Bucci
S
Startup
M
Wynn
P
Baker
A
Lewin
TJ
.
Referential delusions of communication and interpretations of gestures
.
Psychiatry Res
.
2008
;
158
:
27
34
.

8.

Sowden
H
Perkins
M
Clegg
J
.
The co-development of speech and gesture in children with autism
.
Clin Linguist Phon
.
2008
;
22
:
804
813
.

9.

Grüsser
OJ
Kirchhoff
N
Naumann
A
.
Brain mechanisms for recognition of faces, facial expression, and gestures: neuropsychological and electroencephalographic studies in normals, brain-lesioned patients, and schizophrenics
.
Res Publ Assoc Res Nerv Ment Dis
.
1990
;
67
:
165
193
.

10.

Matthews
N
Gold
BJ
Sekuler
R
Park
S
.
Gesture imitation in schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Bull
.
2013
;
39
:
94
101
.

11.

Walther
S
Vanbellingen
T
Müri
R
Strik
W
Bohlhalter
S
.
Impaired pantomime in schizophrenia: association with frontal lobe function
.
Cortex
.
2013
;
49
:
520
527
.

12.

Park
S
Matthews
N
Gibson
C
.
Imitation, simulation, and schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Bull
.
2008
;
34
:
698
707
.

13.

Troisi
A
Spalletta
G
Pasini
A
.
Non-verbal behaviour deficits in schizophrenia: an ethological study of drug-free patients
.
Acta Psychiatr Scand
.
1998
;
97
:
109
115
.

14.

Martin
P
Tewesmeier
M
Albers
M
Schmid
G
Scharfetter
C
.
Investigation of gestural and pantomime performance in chronic schizophrenic inpatients
.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci
.
1994
;
244
:
59
64
.

15.

Berndl
K
von Cranach
M
Grüsser
OJ
.
Impairment of perception and recognition of faces, mimic expression and gestures in schizophrenic patients
.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Neurol Sci
.
1986
;
235
:
282
291
.

16.

Stevenson
RA
VanDerKlok
RM
Pisoni
DB
James
TW
.
Discrete neural substrates underlie complementary audiovisual speech integration processes
.
Neuroimage
.
2011
;
55
:
1339
1345
.

17.

Nath
AR
Beauchamp
MS
.
Dynamic changes in superior temporal sulcus connectivity during perception of noisy audiovisual speech
.
J Neurosci
.
2011
;
31
:
1704
1714
.

18.

Werner
S
Noppeney
U
.
Distinct functional contributions of primary sensory and association areas to audiovisual integration in object categorization
.
J Neurosci
.
2010
;
30
:
2662
2675
.

19.

Noppeney
U
Josephs
O
Hocking
J
Price
CJ
Friston
KJ
.
The effect of prior visual information on recognition of speech and sounds
.
Cereb Cortex
.
2008
;
18
:
598
609
.

20.

Holle
H
Obleser
J
Rueschemeyer
SA
Gunter
TC
.
Integration of iconic gestures and speech in left superior temporal areas boosts speech comprehension under adverse listening conditions
.
Neuroimage
.
2010
;
49
:
875
884
.

21.

Holle
H
Gunter
TC
Rüschemeyer
SA
Hennenlotter
A
Iacoboni
M
.
Neural correlates of the processing of co-speech gestures
.
Neuroimage
.
2008
;
39
:
2010
2024
.

22.

Straube
B
Green
A
Bromberger
B
Kircher
T
.
The differentiation of iconic and metaphoric gestures: common and unique integration processes
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2011
;
32
:
520
533
.

23.

Green
A
Straube
B
Weis
S
et al.  .
Neural integration of iconic and unrelated coverbal gestures: a functional MRI study
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2009
;
30
:
3309
3324
.

24.

Szycik
GR
Münte
TF
Dillo
W
et al.  .
Audiovisual integration of speech is disturbed in schizophrenia: an fMRI study
.
Schizophr Res
.
2009
;
110
:
111
118
.

25.

Surguladze
SA
Calvert
GA
Brammer
MJ
et al.  .
Audio-visual speech perception in schizophrenia: an fMRI study
.
Psychiatry Res
.
2001
;
106
:
1
14
.

26.

de Jong
JJ
Hodiamont
PP
Van den Stock
J
de Gelder
B
.
Audiovisual emotion recognition in schizophrenia: reduced integration of facial and vocal affect
.
Schizophr Res
.
2009
;
107
:
286
293
.

27.

Pearl
D
Yodashkin-Porat
D
Katz
N
et al.  .
Differences in audiovisual integration, as measured by McGurk phenomenon, among adult and adolescent patients with schizophrenia and age-matched healthy control groups
.
Compr Psychiatry
.
2009
;
50
:
186
192
.

28.

Ross
LA
Saint-Amour
D
Leavitt
VM
Molholm
S
Javitt
DC
Foxe
JJ
.
Impaired multisensory processing in schizophrenia: deficits in the visual enhancement of speech comprehension under noisy environmental conditions
.
Schizophr Res
.
2007
;
97
:
173
183
.

29.

de Gelder
B
Vroomen
J
de Jong
SJ
Masthoff
ED
Trompenaars
FJ
Hodiamont
P
.
Multisensory integration of emotional faces and voices in schizophrenics
.
Schizophr Res
.
2005
;
72
:
195
203
.

30.

Abdul-Rahman
MF
Qiu
A
Woon
PS
Kuswanto
C
Collinson
SL
Sim
K
.
Arcuate fasciculus abnormalities and their relationship with psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia
.
PLoS One
.
2012
;
7
:
e29315
.

31.

Kitis
O
Ozalay
O
Zengin
EB
et al.  .
Reduced left uncinate fasciculus fractional anisotropy in deficit schizophrenia but not in non-deficit schizophrenia
.
Psychiatry Clin Neurosci
.
2012
;
66
:
34
43
.

32.

Zalesky
A
Fornito
A
Seal
ML
et al.  .
Disrupted axonal fiber connectivity in schizophrenia
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2011
;
69
:
80
89
.

33.

Catani
M
Craig
MC
Forkel
SJ
et al.  .
Altered integrity of perisylvian language pathways in schizophrenia: relationship to auditory hallucinations
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2011
;
70
:
1143
1150
.

34.

Alexander-Bloch
AF
Vértes
PE
Stidd
R
et al.  .
The anatomical distance of functional connections predicts brain network topology in health and schizophrenia
.
Cereb Cortex
.
2013
;
23
:
127
138
.

35.

Shinn
AK
Baker
JT
Cohen
BM
Ongür
D
.
Functional connectivity of left Heschl’s gyrus in vulnerability to auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Res
.
2013
;
143
:
260
268
.

36.

Bleich-Cohen
M
Sharon
H
Weizman
R
Poyurovsky
M
Faragian
S
Hendler
T
.
Diminished language lateralization in schizophrenia corresponds to impaired inter-hemispheric functional connectivity
.
Schizophr Res
.
2012
;
134
:
131
136
.

37.

Curcic-Blake
B
Liemburg
E
Vercammen
A
et al.  .
When Broca goes uninformed: reduced information flow to Broca’s area in schizophrenia patients with auditory hallucinations
[published online ahead of print October 15,
2012
].
Schizophr Bull.
doi:10.1093/schbul/sbs107.

38.

Liemburg
EJ
Vercammen
A
Ter Horst
GJ
Curcic-Blake
B
Knegtering
H
Aleman
A
.
Abnormal connectivity between attentional, language and auditory networks in schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Res
.
2012
;
135
:
15
22
.

39.

Venkataraman
A
Whitford
TJ
Westin
CF
Golland
P
Kubicki
M
.
Whole brain resting state functional connectivity abnormalities in schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Res
.
2012
;
139
:
7
12
.

40.

Zhou
B
Tan
C
Tang
J
Chen
X
.
Brain functional connectivity of functional magnetic resonance imaging of patients with early-onset schizophrenia
.
Zhong Nan Da Xue Xue Bao Yi Xue Ban
.
2010
;
35
:
17
24
.

41.

Ford
JM
Mathalon
DH
Whitfield
S
Faustman
WO
Roth
WT
.
Reduced communication between frontal and temporal lobes during talking in schizophrenia
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2002
;
51
:
485
492
.

42.

Wolf
DH
Gur
RC
Valdez
JN
et al.  .
Alterations of fronto-temporal connectivity during word encoding in schizophrenia
.
Psychiatry Res
.
2007
;
154
:
221
232
.

43.

Pettersson-Yeo
W
Allen
P
Benetti
S
McGuire
P
Mechelli
A
.
Dysconnectivity in schizophrenia: where are we now?
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2011
;
35
:
1110
1124
.

44.

Kircher
T
Straube
B
Leube
D
et al.  .
Neural interaction of speech and gesture: differential activations of metaphoric co-verbal gestures
.
Neuropsychologia
.
2009
;
47
:
169
179
.

45.

de Bonis
M
Epelbaum
C
Deffez
V
Féline
A
.
The comprehension of metaphors in schizophrenia
.
Psychopathology
.
1997
;
30
:
149
154
.

46.

Kircher
TT
Leube
DT
Erb
M
Grodd
W
Rapp
AM
.
Neural correlates of metaphor processing in schizophrenia
.
Neuroimage
.
2007
;
34
:
281
289
.

47.

Straube
B
Green
A
Sass
K
Kirner-Veselinovic
A
Kircher
T
.
Neural integration of speech and gesture in schizophrenia: Evidence for differential processing of metaphoric gestures
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2013
;
34
:
1696
1712
.

48.

Mittal
VA
Tessner
KD
McMillan
AL
Delawalla
Z
Trotman
HD
Walker
EF
.
Gesture behavior in unmedicated schizotypal adolescents
.
J Abnorm Psychol
.
2006
;
115
:
351
358
.

49.

Spoletini
I
Cherubini
A
Di Paola
M
et al.  .
Reduced fronto-temporal connectivity is associated with frontal gray matter density reduction and neuropsychological deficit in schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Res
.
2009
;
108
:
57
68
.

50.

Crossley
NA
Mechelli
A
Fusar-Poli
P
et al.  .
Superior temporal lobe dysfunction and frontotemporal dysconnectivity in subjects at risk of psychosis and in first-episode psychosis
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2009
;
30
:
4129
4137
.

51.

Oldfield
RC
.
The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory
.
Neuropsychologia
.
1971
;
9
:
97
113
.

52.

Kay
SR
Fiszbein
A
Opler
LA
.
The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Bull
.
1987
;
13
:
261
276
.

53.

Andreasen
NC
Pressler
M
Nopoulos
P
Miller
D
Ho
BC
.
Antipsychotic dose equivalents and dose-years: a standardized method for comparing exposure to different drugs
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2010
;
67
:
255
262
.

54.

Slotnick
SD
Moo
LR
Segal
JB
Hart
J
Jr
.
Distinct prefrontal cortex activity associated with item memory and source memory for visual shapes
.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res
.
2003
;
17
:
75
82
.

55.

Eickhoff
SB
Stephan
KE
Mohlberg
H
et al.  .
A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data
.
Neuroimage
.
2005
;
25
:
1325
1335
.

56.

Annen
S
Roser
P
Brüne
M
.
Nonverbal behavior during clinical interviews: similarities and dissimilarities among schizophrenia, mania, and depression
.
J Nerv Ment Dis
.
2012
;
200
:
26
32
.

57.

Bucci
S
Startup
M
Wynn
P
Heathcote
A
Baker
A
Lewin
TJ
.
Referential delusions of communication and reality discrimination deficits in psychosis
.
Br J Clin Psychol
.
2008
;
47
:
323
334
.

58.

Lavelle
M
Healey
PG
McCabe
R
.
Is nonverbal communi cation disrupted in interactions involving patients with schizophrenia?
[published online ahead of print September 01,
2012
].
Schizophr Bull.
doi:10.1093/schbul/sbs091.

59.

Bergemann
N
Parzer
P
Jaggy
S
Auler
B
Mundt
C
Maier-Braunleder
S
.
Estrogen and comprehension of metaphoric speech in women suffering from schizophrenia: results of a double-blind, placebo-controlled trial
.
Schizophr Bull
.
2008
;
34
:
1172
1181
.

60.

Iakimova
G
Passerieux
C
Denhière
G
et al.  .
The influence of idiomatic salience during the comprehension of ambiguous idioms by patients with schizophrenia
.
Psychiatry Res
.
2010
;
177
:
46
54
.

61.

Calvert
GA
.
Crossmodal processing in the human brain: insights from functional neuroimaging studies
.
Cereb Cortex
.
2001
;
11
:
1110
1123
.

62.

Beauchamp
MS
Argall
BD
Bodurka
J
Duyn
JH
Martin
A
.
Unraveling multisensory integration: patchy organization within human STS multisensory cortex
.
Nat Neurosci
.
2004
;
7
:
1190
1192
.

63.

Dick
AS
Goldin-Meadow
S
Hasson
U
Skipper
JI
Small
SL
.
Co-speech gestures influence neural activity in brain regions associated with processing semantic information
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2009
;
30
:
3509
3526
.

64.

Silverstein
SM
Berten
S
Essex
B
Kovács
I
Susmaras
T
Little
DM
.
An fMRI examination of visual integration in schizophrenia
.
J Integr Neurosci
.
2009
;
8
:
175
202
.

65.

Rizzolatti
G
Sinigaglia
C
.
The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations
.
Nat Rev Neurosci
.
2010
;
11
:
264
274
.

66.

Bernal
B
Ardila
A
.
The role of the arcuate fasciculus in conduction aphasia
.
Brain
.
2009
;
132
:
2309
2316
.

67.

Douaud
G
Smith
S
Jenkinson
M
et al.  .
Anatomically related grey and white matter abnormalities in adolescent-onset schizophrenia
.
Brain
.
2007
;
130
:
2375
2386
.

68.

Kawashima
T
Nakamura
M
Bouix
S
et al.  .
Uncinate fasciculus abnormalities in recent onset schizophrenia and affective psychosis: a diffusion tensor imaging study
.
Schizophr Res
.
2009
;
110
:
119
126
.

69.

McIntosh
AM
Muñoz Maniega
S
Lymer
GK
et al.  .
White matter tractography in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia
.
Biol Psychiatry
.
2008
;
64
:
1088
1092
.

70.

Hubl
D
Koenig
T
Strik
W
et al.  .
Pathways that make voices: white matter changes in auditory hallucinations
.
Arch Gen Psychiatry
.
2004
;
61
:
658
668
.

71.

Jeong
B
Wible
CG
Hashimoto
R
Kubicki
M
.
Functional and anatomical connectivity abnormalities in left inferior frontal gyrus in schizophrenia
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2009
;
30
:
4138
4151
.

72.

Mitchell
RL
Crow
TJ
.
Right hemisphere language functions and schizophrenia: the forgotten hemisphere?
Brain
.
2005
;
128
:
963
978
.

73.

Camchong
J
MacDonald
AW
3rd
Bell
C
Mueller
BA
Lim
KO
.
Altered functional and anatomical connectivity in schizophrenia
.
Schizophr Bull
.
2011
;
37
:
640
650
.

74.

Bluhm
RL
Miller
J
Lanius
RA
et al.  .
Spontaneous low-frequency fluctuations in the BOLD signal in schizophrenic patients: anomalies in the default network
.
Schizophr Bull
.
2007
;
33
:
1004
1012
.

75.

Whitfield-Gabrieli
S
Thermenos
HW
Milanovic
S
et al.  .
Hyperactivity and hyperconnectivity of the default network in schizophrenia and in first-degree relatives of persons with schizophrenia
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
.
2009
;
106
:
1279
1284
.

76.

Salvador
R
Sarró
S
Gomar
JJ
et al.  .
Overall brain connectivity maps show cortico-subcortical abnormalities in schizophrenia
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2010
;
31
:
2003
2014
.

77.

Mannell
MV
Franco
AR
Calhoun
VD
Cañive
JM
Thoma
RJ
Mayer
AR
.
Resting state and task-induced deactivation: a methodological comparison in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
2010
;
31
:
424
437
.

78.

Thompson
LA
Guzman
FA
.
Some limits on encoding visible speech and gestures using a dichotic shadowing task
.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci
.
1999
;
54
:
P347
P349
.

Supplementary data